Article

Proximate factors mediating ‘contact’ calls in adult female baboons (Papio cynocephalus ursinus) and their infants. J Comp Psychol

Department of Psychology, University of Pennsylvania, USA.
Journal of Comparative Psychology (Impact Factor: 2.31). 04/2000; 114(1):36-46. DOI: 10.1037/0735-7036.114.1.36
Source: PubMed

ABSTRACT "Contact" calls are widespread in social mammals and birds, but the proximate factors that motivate call production and mediate their contact function remain poorly specified. Field study of chacma baboons (Papio cynocephalus ursinus) revealed that contact barks in adult females were motivated by separation both from the group at large and from their dependent infants. A variety of social and ecological factors affect the probability of separation from either one or both. Results of simultaneous observations and a playback experiment indicate that the contact function of calling between mothers and infants was mediated by occasional maternal retrieval rather than coordinated call exchange. Mothers recognized the contact barks of their own infants and often were strongly motivated to locate them. However, mothers did not produce contact barks in reply unless they themselves were at risk of becoming separated from the group.

Download full-text

Full-text

Available from: Dorothy L. Cheney, Oct 30, 2014
0 Followers
 · 
99 Views
  • Source
    • "In addition, selective pressure to understand the signals in these social networks may have contributed to selection for increased cognitive abilities in primates (Altenmüller et al. 2013; Barrett et al. 2003; de Waal and Tyack 2003; Dunbar 1998, 2003a, b; Dunbar and Shultz 2007; Pollard and Blumstein 2011, 2012). Previous studies have shown that the recognition of individuals by voice is common in nonprimate mammals: elephants (McComb et al. 2000), hyenas (Holekamp et al. 1999), pinnepeds (Insley 2001; Insley et al. 2003), bats (Balcombe 1990; Balcombe and McCracken 1992; Kastein et al. 2013; Knoernschild and Von Helversen 2008), rodents (Pollard and Blumstein 2011), and in haplorrhine primates: rhesus monkeys (Rendall et al. 1996), baboons (Cheney and Seyfarth 1999; Rendall et al. 2000), vervets (Cheney and Seyfarth 1980), marmosets (Snowdon and Cleveland 1980), and squirrel monkeys (Symmes and Biben 1985). In contrast, very little is known about vocal recognition in strepsirrhine primates, e.g., maternal recognition of infants in ring-tailed lemurs (Nunn 2000) and recognition of fathers by daughters in mouse lemurs (Kessler et al. 2012). "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Social complexity is argued to be a driving factor in the evolution of communicative complexity. Complex social systems require individuals to form relationships with many conspecifics and interact in a wide variety of contexts over time, thus selecting for acoustic communication systems complex enough to facilitate these relationships. To better understand the evolution of such social and communicative complexity, we investigated a nocturnal, solitary forager, Garnett’s bushbaby (Otolemur garnettii), as a lorisoid model for the ancestral primate social systems from which more complex systems evolved.We hypothesized that it would be advantageous for solitary foragers to have individual differences in long-distance calls, as this could be crucial to the maintenance of their dispersed social networks. We tested for individual differences in the long distance bark vocalization. We measured 6 frequency and temporal parameters for 120 barks (15 barks from each of 8 individuals housed at the University of Southern Mississippi). Principal component and discriminant function analyses assigned the calls to the respective individuals at a rate that was moderately accurate and higher than chance (binomial test: 54.2% correct, P < 0.001, chance = 12.5%). This pilot work provides moderate evidence for individual differences and isthe first such study to be conducted on lorisoids. Because individual differences have been documented in the vocalizations of solitary foraging lemuroids, we suggest that moderate individual differences may have been present in ancestral primates and contributed to the dispersed social system that is thought to have been the foundation from which increased social complexity evolved in primates.
    International Journal of Primatology 06/2015; DOI:10.1007/s10764-015-9847-z · 1.99 Impact Factor
  • Source
    • "If they locate the general whereabouts of the entire group, they may change behaviour in relation to their own spatial position, for example, central– peripheral zone of the group (Robinson 1981; Janson 1990). Further examination of behavioural changes in relation to relative position of the group is necessary , such as monitoring behaviour of group members (Kazahari & Agetsuma 2010; Suzuki & Sugiura 2011) and contact calls (Boinski & Campbell 1995; Rendall et al. 2000; this study). "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: A well-known behavioural model for group aggregation is that an individual depends on a few neighbouring individuals to adjust its movement, such as departure (repulsion) from and approach (attraction) to neighbours. However, an individual may rely not only on a few closest neighbours, but also on more distant individuals, in a group of stable membership. We measured temporal changes in the local density of individuals around a focal individual and changes in distance to other focal individuals in a group of wild Japanese macaques to determine whether the macaques depended only on a few neighbours or also on more distant individuals for adjustments in cohesiveness. We used simultaneous focal animal sampling, with two observers recording the individuals' locations using a global positioning system (GPS), over three seasons. Numbers of individuals within 20 m from an animal tended to increase after 10 min when there were a small number of individuals around the animal. However, the number tended to decrease when there was a larger number of individuals. It remained similar when there were an intermediate number of individuals. The two focal animals tended to separate after 10 min when the interindividual distance was short. However, they tended to move closer when far apart. They remained a similar distance apart when they were at an intermediate distance. Contact calls, which are suggested to function as locating group members and keeping cohesiveness, were emitted more frequently when the distance between the two focal animals was very large in two seasons. However, the rate of contact calls was not influenced by the number of individuals within 20 m from an animal. These results suggest that individual Japanese macaques do not only rely on a few closest neighbours, but also rely on more distant group members. Japanese macaques may know the general whereabouts of the whole group, and when they stay at the periphery of the group, they may emit contact calls frequently and move towards the central zone so as not to become separated from the group.
    Ethology 01/2014; 120(1). DOI:10.1111/eth.12176 · 1.56 Impact Factor
  • Source
    • "Marler et al. 1992). However, while there is debate about whether the signaller intends to inform (Cheney & Seyfarth 1990; Cheney et al.1996; Tomasello & Call 1997; Rendall et al. 2000) and about the exact definition of the information content (Rendall et al. 2009), there is growing evidence for elaborate inferring mechanisms in receivers (Fischer 1988; Rendall et al. 1996; Zuberbühler et al. 1999). Most studies so far have focused on animal species that produce alarm calls in the context of predation and thus possess cognitive abilities that are involved in both signalling and perceiving information about predator presence. "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Advanced inferring abilities that are used for predator recognition and avoidance have been documented in a variety of animal species that produce alarm calls. In contrast, evidence for cognitive abilities that underpin predation avoidance in nonalarm-calling species is restricted to associative learning of heterospecific alarm calls and predator presence. We investigated cognitive capacities that underlie the perception and computation of external information beyond associative learning by addressing contextual information processing in pigeons, Columba livia, a bird species without specific alarm calls. We used a habituation/dishabituation paradigm across sensory modes to test pigeons' context-dependent inferring abilities. The birds reliably took previous knowledge about predator presence into account and responded with predator-specific scanning behaviour only if predator presence was not indicated before or if the perceived level of urgency increased. Hence, pigeons' antipredator behaviour was not based on the physical properties of displayed stimuli or their referential content alone but on contextual information, indicated by the kind and order of stimulus presentation and different sensory modes.
    Animal Behaviour 03/2013; 85(3):605-613. DOI:10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.12.023 · 3.14 Impact Factor
Show more