Article

Psychometrics of odor quality discrimination: method for threshold determination.

Department of Psychology, Uppsala University, Box 1225, S-751 42 Uppsala, Sweden.
Chemical Senses (Impact Factor: 3.22). 11/2000; 25(5):493-9. DOI: 10.1093/chemse/25.5.493
Source: PubMed

ABSTRACT There is no natural physical continuum for odor quality along which an odor quality discrimination (OQD) threshold can be measured. In an attempt to overcome this problem, the substitution-reciprocity (SURE) method defines a framework for the measurement of an OQD threshold. More specifically, it (i) defines a threshold concept for OQD, including the quantification of qualitative change of the stimulus, and (ii) suggests how to avoid perceived intensity as an unwanted cue for discrimination. In doing this, the psychometric properties of odor quality in the case of eugenol and citral are investigated using both discrimination (experiment 1) and scaling (experiment 2). Based on discriminatory responses, a change of approximately one-third in stimulus content was needed to reach the OQD threshold for eugenol and citral.

0 Bookmarks
 · 
54 Views
  • [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: The aim was to compare risk factors for stroke and ischemic heart disease (IHD) in a large general population cohort. A prospective cohort of 82,380 participants (aged 55.4 [SD 14.2 yrs], 44.8% men) without known history of cardiovascular diseases (CVD) at baseline was pooled from ten years (1994-2004) of the Health Survey for England. Study members were followed, on average, over 8 years for cause-specific mortality using linkage to national registers. There were 806 and 1346 stroke and IHD deaths, respectively. The major risk factors for stroke included age (hazard ratio [HR] = 1.15, 95% CI, 1.13-1.17), smoking (HR = 1.71; 1.20-2.44), diabetes (HR = 1.75; 1.05-2.93), total cholesterol (HR per SD = 0.78; 0.69-0.89), and systolic BP (HR per SD = 1.22; 1.08-1.38). In addition to these risk factors, IHD was also predicted by high density lipoprotein cholesterol, body mass index, C-reactive protein, and fibrinogen. This pattern of results was consistent among younger (<70 yrs) and older adults. In a large representative cohort of the general population we found a differential pattern of risk markers for stroke compared with IHD. This was not explained by differences in age at onset of disease.
    Atherosclerosis 08/2011; 219(2):807-10. · 3.71 Impact Factor
  • [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Our olfactory system is confronted with complex mixtures of odorants, often recognized as single entities due to odor blending (e.g., coffee). In contrast, we are also able to discriminate odors from complex mixtures (e.g., off-odors). Therefore, the olfactory system is able to engage either configural or elemental processes when confronted with mixtures. However, the rules that govern the involvement of these processes during odor perception remain poorly understood. In our first experiment, we examined whether simple odorant mixtures (binary/ternary) could elicit configural perception. Twenty untrained subjects were asked to evaluate the odor typicality of mixtures and their constituents. The results revealed a significant increase in odor typicality in some but not all mixtures as compared with the single components, which suggest that perceptual odor blending can occur only in specific mixtures (configural processing). In our second experiment, we tested the hypothesis that general olfactory expertise can improve elemental perception of mixtures. Thirty-two trained subjects evaluated the odor typicality of the stimuli presented during the first experiment, and their responses were compared with those obtained from the untrained panelists. The results support the idea that general training with odors increases the elemental perception of binary and ternary blending mixtures.
    Chemical Senses 08/2011; 37(2):159-66. · 3.22 Impact Factor
  • Source
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: Processing of odor mixtures by neonates is weakly understood. Previous studies showed that a binary mixture of ethyl isobutyrate/ethyl maltol (odorants A/B) blends in newborn rabbits at the 30/70 ratio: Pups would perceive a configural odor in addition to the components' odors. Here, we investigated whether the emergence of this additional odor in AB is determined by specific ratio(s) of A and B. To that goal, we tested whether pups discriminated between AB mixtures with lower (A(-)B, 8/92 ratio) or higher (A(+)B, 68/32) proportion of A. In Experiment 1, pups conditioned to A (or B) responded to A(-)B and A(+)B but not to AB. In Experiment 2, pups responded to A(-)B after learning of A(-) (and to A(+)B after learning of A(+)) but not to AB. In Experiment 3, after conditioning to A(-)B pups responded to A(-) and B (and to A(+) and B after learning of A(+)B) but not or less to AB. In Experiment 4, pups responded to A(-)B and A(+)B after conditioning to AB. These results confirm the configural perception of certain odor mixtures by young organisms and reveal that the proportion of components is a key factor influencing their coding, recognition, and discrimination of complex stimuli.
    Chemical Senses 05/2011; 36(8):693-700. · 3.22 Impact Factor

Full-text

View
1 Download