Photopigment basis for dichromatic color vision in the horse

Department of Cell Biology, Neurobiology & Anatomy, Medical College of Wisconsin, Milwaukee, WI, USA.
Journal of Vision (Impact Factor: 2.39). 08/2001; 1(2):80-7. DOI: 10.1167/1.2.2
Source: PubMed


Horses, like other ungulates, are active in the day, at dusk, dawn, and night; and, they have eyes designed to have both high sensitivity for vision in dim light and good visual acuity under higher light levels (Walls, 1942). Typically, daytime activity is associated with the presence of multiple cone classes and color-vision capacity (Jacobs, 1993). Previous studies in other ungulates, such as pigs, goats, cows, sheep and deer, have shown that they have two spectrally different cone types, and hence, at least the photopigment basis for dichromatic color vision (Neitz & Jacobs, 1989; Jacobs, Deegan II, Neitz, Murphy, Miller, & Marchinton, 1994; Jacobs, Deegan II, & Neitz, 1998). Here, electroretinogram flicker photometry was used to measure the spectral sensitivities of the cones in the domestic horse (Equus caballus). Two distinct spectral mechanisms were identified and are consistent with the presence of a short-wavelength-sensitive (S) and a middle-to-long-wavelength-sensitive (M/L) cone. The spectral sensitivity of the S cone was estimated to have a peak of 428 nm, while the M/L cone had a peak of 539 nm. These two cone types would provide the basis for dichromatic color vision consistent with recent results from behavioral testing of horses (Macuda & Timney, 1999; Macuda & Timney, 2000; Timney & Macuda, 2001). The spectral peak of the M/L cone photopigment measured here, in vivo, is similar to that obtained when the gene was sequenced, cloned, and expressed in vitro (Yokoyama & Radlwimmer, 1999). Of the ungulates that have been studied to date, all have the photopigment basis for dichromatic color vision; however, they differ considerably from one another in the spectral tuning of their cone pigments. These differences may represent adaptations to the different visual requirements of different species.

Download full-text


Available from: Christopher J Murphy,
  • Source
    • "Previous studies using different methods indicated that the horse has two cone pigments (Sandmann et al., 1996; Carroll et al., 2001; Hall et al., 2006). Carroll et al. (2001) reported that the cone spectral sensitivity for the M/L photopigment of the horses has a spectral peak (λmax) of 539 nm and that the best fitting S cone pigment curve had a λmax "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: There is general agreement that horses have dichromatic colour vision with similar capabilities to human beings with red-green colour deficiencies. However, whether colour perception has an impact on equine jumping performance and how pronounced the colour stimulus might be for a horse is unknown. The present study investigated the relationship between the colour of the fences (blue or green) and the show jumping performance of 20 horses ridden by two riders using an indoor and outdoor set of green and blue fences. In the indoor arena, significantly more touches and faults were made on blue fences in comparison to green fences (median difference of 2.5 bars). When only touched bars were included, a significant median difference of 1 bar was found. Mares (n = 4) demonstrated more faults and had a significantly greater difference in touches and faults between the two colours than male horses (n = 16). Repeating the same experiment with eight horses in an outdoor grass arena revealed no significant differences between the two colours. In order to draw any definite conclusions, more research concerning the colour perception, influence of contrast with the arena surface and sex of horse is required.
    The Veterinary Journal 10/2014; DOI:10.1016/j.tvjl.2014.07.016 · 1.76 Impact Factor
  • Source
    • "Scientifically, this interest focuses on the gathering of as much data as possible about a particular sensory ability of a species, which then provides important information to the horse public regarding the visual abilities of their animals. Investigations to date centered on equine color vision (Pick et al., 1994; Macuda and Timney, 1999; Carroll et al., 2001; Hanggi et al., 2007), depth perception and visual acuity (see Timney and Macuda, 2001; Waring, 2003 for reviews), interocular transfer (Hanggi, 1999a), and scotopic vision (Hanggi and Ingersoll, 2009a). The horse's field of view is another aspect of equine vision that has been the subject of some discussion. "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: This study investigated lateral vision in horses (Equus caballus) for the first time from a behavioral point of view. Three horses were tested using a novel experimental design to determine the range of their lateral and caudolateral vision with respect to stimulus detection and discrimination. Real-life stimuli were presented along a curvilinear wall in one of four different positions (A, B, C, D) and one of two height locations (Top, Bottom) on both sides of the horse. To test for stimulus detection, the correct stimulus was paired against a control; for stimulus discrimination, the correct stimulus was paired against another object. To indicate that the correct stimulus was detected or discriminated, the horses pushed one of two paddles. All horses scored significantly above chance on stimulus detection trials regardless of stimulus position or location. They also accurately discriminated between stimuli when objects appeared in positions A, B, and C for the top or bottom locations; however, they failed to discriminate these stimuli at position D. This study supports physiological descriptions of the equine eye and provides new behavioral data showing that horses can detect the appearance of objects within an almost fully encompassing circle and are able to identify objects within most but not all of their panoramic field of view.
    Behavioural processes 06/2012; 91(1):70-6. DOI:10.1016/j.beproc.2012.05.009 · 1.57 Impact Factor
  • Source
    • "The color vision of locusts is based on three types of photoreceptor with peak sensitivities at 360, 430 and 530 nm (Briscoe and Chittka 2001; Vishnevskaya and Shura-Bura 1990). Color vision in goats (as well as many other ungulates) is dichromatic, with receptor peak sensitivities at 443 and 553 nm (Jacobs et al. 1998; Carroll et al. 2001). The ability of animals to discriminate between color signals depends on the difference in excitation of the color receptors by each signal, and on receptor noise (Osorio and Vorobyev 2005). "
    [Show abstract] [Hide abstract]
    ABSTRACT: The evolution of several floral traits is thought to be driven by multiple selective agents, including pollinators and herbivores. Similar combinations of selection pressures may have shaped extra-floral traits. The conspicuous purple tufts of leaves (“flags”), which often terminate vertical inflorescences in the Mediterranean annual Salvia viridis, were shown to attract insect pollinators to the flowering patch. Here we test whether they also function as anti-herbivore signals. We determined the aposematic potential of S. viridis flags on three levels: concentrations of anthocyanins, suggested to function as aposematic visual signals, in leaves and flags; spectrometry to estimate whether the color-vision system of two common Mediterranean generalist herbivores (locusts and goats) can discriminate flags from leaves; and choice experiments to determine food preferences of the same herbivores. Anthocyanin concentrations in flags were >10-fold higher than in leaves. Flags exhibited peak reflectance at 450 and 700 nm wavelengths, while leaves reflected maximally at 550 nm. According to the Vorobyev-Osorio color vision model, these differences in color reflection are likely to allow visual discrimination by herbivores. Goats preferred feeding on clipped inflorescences over control inflorescences. Locusts preferred leaves over flags. To test whether this was due to deterrence from the flags’ coloration, we also offered them choice between leaves and a rare, white morph, of the flags. The locusts chose both equally immediately after presentation, but leaves attracted more individuals after 5 min of feeding. The locusts also preferred green cabbage over anthocyanin-rich red cabbage. These results support the function of colorful extra-floral displays as warning signals.
    Evolutionary Ecology 05/2012; 26(3):499-512. DOI:10.1007/s10682-011-9509-3 · 2.52 Impact Factor
Show more